Remembering That Reading is "A Way of Happening.".

Sandra M. Murphy
{"title":"Remembering That Reading is \"A Way of Happening.\".","authors":"Sandra M. Murphy","doi":"10.1080/00098659809599603","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"When I was just beginning graduate studies, I became enthralled by what for me was a new way of thinking about the transition that children make when they go to school to learn to read. At the time, I was reading articles that said that the move from the familiar and essentially oral world of the home to the literate world of the school was one that required a shift from interpretive strategies of daily discourse in favor of new \"logical\" and \"literal\" approaches to interpretation (e.g., Olson 1977; Olson and Hildyard 1983). In this view, learning to read and write required formal education in which children learned to decode and interpret what were then called \"autonomous\" texts-texts that were self-contained systems of words that supposedly carried meaning independently of the social contexts in which they existed-by learning about the conventions of written language, semantics, the forms of texts, and the regularities of alphabetic writing. They learned, that is, by focusing on the forms and structures of language. Although I didn't have any personal memories of learning to read (it seemed to have happened naturally before I went to school), I was enamored with what I was learning, in part because it seemed to explain to me why learning to read seemed to be so hard for some children. After all, studying abstract things like language demanded extraordinary powers. (I believed this, I now think, because at the time I was having troubles of my own struggling with language \"in the abstract\" in several courses on linguistics.) I was also immersed in reading about models of the reading process. At the time, only a few models included much of anything about emotions or feelings. One exception was a model proposed by Robert Ruddell (1976), who wrote about \"affective mobilizers\"-in other words, a reader's beliefs, values, and attitudes-and the way these \"mobilizers\"","PeriodicalId":339545,"journal":{"name":"The Clearing House","volume":"45 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1998-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"14","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The Clearing House","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/00098659809599603","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14

Abstract

When I was just beginning graduate studies, I became enthralled by what for me was a new way of thinking about the transition that children make when they go to school to learn to read. At the time, I was reading articles that said that the move from the familiar and essentially oral world of the home to the literate world of the school was one that required a shift from interpretive strategies of daily discourse in favor of new "logical" and "literal" approaches to interpretation (e.g., Olson 1977; Olson and Hildyard 1983). In this view, learning to read and write required formal education in which children learned to decode and interpret what were then called "autonomous" texts-texts that were self-contained systems of words that supposedly carried meaning independently of the social contexts in which they existed-by learning about the conventions of written language, semantics, the forms of texts, and the regularities of alphabetic writing. They learned, that is, by focusing on the forms and structures of language. Although I didn't have any personal memories of learning to read (it seemed to have happened naturally before I went to school), I was enamored with what I was learning, in part because it seemed to explain to me why learning to read seemed to be so hard for some children. After all, studying abstract things like language demanded extraordinary powers. (I believed this, I now think, because at the time I was having troubles of my own struggling with language "in the abstract" in several courses on linguistics.) I was also immersed in reading about models of the reading process. At the time, only a few models included much of anything about emotions or feelings. One exception was a model proposed by Robert Ruddell (1976), who wrote about "affective mobilizers"-in other words, a reader's beliefs, values, and attitudes-and the way these "mobilizers"
记住阅读是“发生的一种方式”。
当我刚开始研究生学习的时候,我被一种新的思考方式所吸引,这种方式是关于孩子们在学校学习阅读时所经历的转变。当时,我读到的一些文章说,从熟悉的、本质上是口头的家庭世界向学校的文学世界的转变,需要从日常话语的解释策略转向新的“逻辑”和“字面”的解释方法(例如,Olson 1977;Olson and Hildyard 1983)。在这种观点中,学习阅读和写作需要正规的教育,通过学习书面语言的惯例、语义、文本的形式和字母书写的规律,孩子们学会解码和解释当时被称为“自主”的文本——这些文本是独立于它们所处的社会环境而承载意义的自足的单词系统。也就是说,他们通过关注语言的形式和结构来学习。虽然我对学习阅读没有任何个人记忆(这似乎是在我上学之前自然而然发生的),但我对我正在学习的东西很着迷,部分原因是它似乎向我解释了为什么学习阅读对一些孩子来说如此困难。毕竟,学习语言这样抽象的东西需要非凡的能力。(我现在认为,我相信这一点,因为当时我自己在几门语言学课程上也遇到了“抽象”语言的问题。)我也沉浸在阅读过程的模型中。当时,只有少数模型包含了很多关于情绪或感觉的东西。一个例外是Robert Ruddell(1976)提出的一个模型,他写了“情感动员者”——换句话说,读者的信仰、价值观和态度——以及这些“动员者”的方式。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信