From basic social neurobiology to better understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders

IF 4.6 Q2 MATERIALS SCIENCE, BIOMATERIALS
Andrey E. Ryabinin
{"title":"From basic social neurobiology to better understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders","authors":"Andrey E. Ryabinin","doi":"10.1111/gbb.12818","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>This volume concludes our three-part series of Genes, Brain and Behaviors special issues. The previous two volumes started with research on autism and neurodevelopmental disorders, concluding that their full understanding is impossible without understanding of innate social behaviors, and continued into research on mechanisms of such innate behaviors across several animal species.<span><sup>1, 2</sup></span> The current special issue continues the analysis of innate social behaviors and then brings us back to studies on animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders, hopefully with a greater appreciation for intricacies of social neurobiology.</p><p>In their research paper, Goncalves, Kareklas and colleagues have elegantly addressed a fundamental question on evolution: whether the motivational and the cognitive components comprising social behavior evolved independently or through selective pressure to increase sociality.<span><sup>3</sup></span> To address this question, the authors phenotypically characterized several lines of male and female zebrafish (<i>Danio rerio</i>) in four behavioral tests: shoal preference, conspecific recognition, object recognition and open-field. This characterization clustered behaviors across three principal factors: motivation, cognition and anxiety. Importantly, the social tendency module in this analysis aligned with object and social exploration, and clustered separately from object discrimination and social discrimination, which aligned together. These data provide strong support for the hypothesis that social recognition and social motivation did not evolve together as a common phenotype and instead separately co-opted general cognitive and motivational mechanisms. This conclusion is further supported by analysis of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and behavioral modules. The genetic analysis shows that SNPs in candidate genes associated with social behaviors are statistically associated with the motivational but not the cognitive module. This finding is in agreement with the recent report showing that zebrafish lacking functional oxytocin receptors are deficient in social and object recognition but have intact social motivation.<span><sup>4</sup></span> While the well-characterized behavioral repertoire and genetics of zebrafish allows the researchers to conclude that social recognition and social motivation can function as independent entities, this conclusion amplifies the need to study neural mechanisms regulating specific social behaviors, rather than the more generalized “social brain.”</p><p>Not all specific social behaviors, including social behaviors characteristic of humans, can be studied in standard laboratory species. The previous volume of the special issue highlighted the importance of choosing appropriate species for such studies by discussing the power of experiments in socially monogamous prairie voles for understanding the neurobiology of pair-bonding.<span><sup>5, 6</sup></span> The research paper by Mederos, Duarte and colleagues in the current issue initiates the development of a novel fish model that could enhance our understanding of pair-bonding.<span><sup>7</sup></span> The paper is based on the premise of frequently observed social and genetic monogamy across several related species of seahorses,<span><sup>8-10</sup></span> which suggests that they are capable of pair-bonding. In their paper, the authors investigate color changes as a form of social communication in lined seahorses (<i>Hippocampus erectus</i>). They demonstrate that the color change (increase in brightness) occurs more frequently, especially in male fish, when they are paired with their pair mate, than when they are paired with an opposite-sex stranger. In a subsequent preliminary whole brain RNA sequencing analysis authors detect a substantial overlap in pairing-induced changes in gene expression between male seahorses and prairie voles. Remarkably, oxytocin receptor is among upregulated genes in paired female seahorses, in agreement with its well-known importance in pair-bonding in other species.</p><p>Animal species adopt different modes of social communication depending on evolutionary constraints. Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) are of great importance for social communication in rodents. Previous research demonstrated that communication via USVs depends on age, sex and can also differ between mouse strains.<span><sup>11</sup></span> However, the detailed understanding of differences in such communication and its relationship to underlying behavioral situation is missing. In their research paper, Caruso et al.<span><sup>12</sup></span> provide an extensive analysis of vocal repertoire across several early postnatal ages and in adults of three mouse strains: the commonly used inbred C57BL/6N, the highly social inbred FVB and the outbred CD-1. Moreover, the USVs were analyzed across three behavioral situations: social separation in mouse pups, adult male–female encounters and adult female–female encounters. In parallel, the authors assess the behavioral repertoire of adult mice during the interactions. While the USV repertoire of mice changes from early postnatal to adult age, strain- and social context-dependent differences are ever present in all assessments of vocalizations.</p><p>The behavioral repertoire of animals can not only differ between different ages, genotypes and social context, but can be even different between cagemate rodents of the same strain, sex and age. Animals living in groups usually organize themselves into social hierarchies. Behavioral repertoires, as well as general well-being, are different between socially-dominant and subordinate individuals within these groups.<span><sup>13</sup></span> Park and colleagues in their research paper address an intriguing question whether social rank affects empathy-like behavior in mice.<span><sup>14</sup></span> The authors determine social ranks in male C57BL/6J mice using the well-established tube test for social ranking and then evaluate these mice for their ability to develop empathy-like observational fear learning (OFL). In OFL, “observer” mice acquire fear from “demonstrator” mice undergoing fear conditioning. The authors demonstrate that intermediately ranking observers are better at acquiring fear from dominant than from subordinate demonstrators. In the same direction of effect, subordinate observers are better than dominant cagemates at acquiring fear from intermediately ranking demonstrators. Moreover, the OFL is dependent on the steepness of the social hierarchy. These findings are in agreement with a previous study showing that the subordinate rank in rats enhances social fear transmission,<span><sup>15</sup></span> suggesting generality of this effect across species. However, the previous studies were not able to discern whether this increased fear is due to familiarity of the observer with the social rank of the demonstrator or due to some intrinsic quality of dominant demonstrator. Park and colleagues rule out the second possibility by showing that social ranking does not affect OFL from unfamiliar mice. Their study definitively determines higher empathy-like responses in more subordinate versus more dominant cagemates, at least in male mice. While not the main focus of the study, this article also shows that both single-housing and crowding strongly inhibit empathy-like OFL in mice calling to attention the importance of housing conditions for assessing social behaviors.</p><p>The latter observation is well in line with the next article in this special issue. The research paper by Winiarsky, Kondrakiewicz and colleagues compares effects of housing, habituation and testing conditions in males of commonly used C57BL/6J mice versus BTBR T+ Iptr3tf/J mice (BTBR) mice.<span><sup>16</sup></span> The BTBR mice are a recognized idiopathic model of aspects of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and as such demonstrate deficits in social behaviors.<span><sup>17, 18</sup></span> In the first series of experiments, the authors test these mice in the 3-chamber social affiliation test under several low and high stress conditions. These conditions range from testing non-habituated mice under relatively bright light conditions to habituated to handing and transportation mice, housed in enriched environment and tested in dim light. Behavior of the mice changes substantially depending on these conditions, at time obscuring the ability to detect strain differences. In the second series of experiment, the authors use an automated Eco-HAB system,<span><sup>19</sup></span> which allows long-lasting evaluations of group-housed mice without disturbing them, to assess differences in social behaviors between these strains of mice. Among the identified striking differences, C57BL/6J mice demonstrate stability of social structure, whereas only limited subgroups of interactive BTBR mice engage in sporadic social interactions in these settings. Authors discuss the pros and cons for each of the two approaches for evaluating differences in the Systems for Social Processes Domain between mouse strains and highlight how differences in experimental conditions can clearly contribute to varied results.</p><p>The special issue is concluded with two important reviews on neurodevelopmental disorders and their animal models. The review by Benedetti and colleagues takes on one genetic mechanism with high penetrance for such disorders.<span><sup>20</sup></span> Namely, authors screened over 750 publications and analyzed 58 studies in humans and 16 studies in mice that focused on behavioral characterization of subjects with copy number variations (CNVs) in DNA segments 16p11.2 and 22q11.2. CNVs on these genetic loci are associated with substantially increased risk for ASD, schizophrenia and attention deficit disorder.<span><sup>21, 22</sup></span> Mutant mice with deletions of appropriate DNA loci have been developed. These mouse mutants provide models with excellent construct validity for the corresponding CNVs in humans. Authors review deficits in the several social domains in subjects with CMVs (involving social interaction, social communication, social memory and social cognition) and criteria for their identification. They report on several matching changes in tests of sociability, social interaction and social memory and communication (vis USVs) in the mutant mice. These commonalities in observations between human and mouse studies are promising for better understanding of associated disorders. However, on one hand, the number of behavioral tests assessed mice with relevant CNVs are sparse, compared with the vast array of murine social behavioral tests available. On the other hand, even the performed tests do not align well with the psychiatric diagnostic criteria used in humans. While highlighting the deficiencies, Benedetti and colleagues discuss several exciting avenues for further testing in rodent models (for example, assessing recognition of visual cues, parental influences, immunological mechanisms and effects of microbiome).</p><p>While an increase in studies using relevant tests on social behaviors would benefit understanding of deficits in social behaviors associated with CNVs, the final paper in this issue questions how the animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders should be optimally utilized. Silverman and colleagues report on recommendations of the workshop held by the Autism Science Foundation with the goal of improving utilization of such models.<span><sup>23</sup></span> The resulting review collected 148 and analyzed 69 articles on ASD mouse models. The analysis reveals inconsistent reporting patterns in behavioral performance, experimental design and insufficient alignment of behavioral testing with diagnostic criteria of ASD. The authors emphasize the need to focus on construct validity of models, rather than on their face validity, as well as the importance of not underestimating the complexity of behavior in animal models. Silverman and colleagues acknowledge the challenges of translating between mouse and human behaviors, the complication of results due to use of various genetic backgrounds, use of different methodologies across labs, problems with inter-laboratory reproducibility, often insufficiently clear or incorrectly applied statistical analyses, lack of appreciation for temporal trajectories of phenotypes, the need to go beyond mice to represent the full repertoire of complex behaviors and the need to characterize models of neurodevelopmental disorders across multiple behavioral domains. The authors recommend that researchers assess not only social behavior in their models but measure cognitive and motor abilities, assess their models across a spectrum of comorbidities, keep in mind the role of olfaction in rodent social interactions, control for underlying motivation of subjects in their studies, all the while sticking to meticulous reporting of experimental design, detailed methods, materials, results and statistical analyses.</p><p>We conclude this series of special issues on social behaviors, autism and neurodevelopmental disorders by emphasizing that this exciting research field exhibits much motivation to go beyond superficial observations of simplistic phenotypes. The front cover image of this special issue, by Dr. Yangmiao Zhang, illustrates that participants in social interactions come with different personal experiences and backgrounds. These experiences guide them to exhibit various behaviors, including social bonding. May the complexity of social behaviors serve as a common challenge to bond neuroscientists in the pursuit of intricate underlying mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":2,"journal":{"name":"ACS Applied Bio Materials","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6000,"publicationDate":"2022-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9744557/pdf/","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"ACS Applied Bio Materials","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/gbb.12818","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"MATERIALS SCIENCE, BIOMATERIALS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

This volume concludes our three-part series of Genes, Brain and Behaviors special issues. The previous two volumes started with research on autism and neurodevelopmental disorders, concluding that their full understanding is impossible without understanding of innate social behaviors, and continued into research on mechanisms of such innate behaviors across several animal species.1, 2 The current special issue continues the analysis of innate social behaviors and then brings us back to studies on animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders, hopefully with a greater appreciation for intricacies of social neurobiology.

In their research paper, Goncalves, Kareklas and colleagues have elegantly addressed a fundamental question on evolution: whether the motivational and the cognitive components comprising social behavior evolved independently or through selective pressure to increase sociality.3 To address this question, the authors phenotypically characterized several lines of male and female zebrafish (Danio rerio) in four behavioral tests: shoal preference, conspecific recognition, object recognition and open-field. This characterization clustered behaviors across three principal factors: motivation, cognition and anxiety. Importantly, the social tendency module in this analysis aligned with object and social exploration, and clustered separately from object discrimination and social discrimination, which aligned together. These data provide strong support for the hypothesis that social recognition and social motivation did not evolve together as a common phenotype and instead separately co-opted general cognitive and motivational mechanisms. This conclusion is further supported by analysis of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and behavioral modules. The genetic analysis shows that SNPs in candidate genes associated with social behaviors are statistically associated with the motivational but not the cognitive module. This finding is in agreement with the recent report showing that zebrafish lacking functional oxytocin receptors are deficient in social and object recognition but have intact social motivation.4 While the well-characterized behavioral repertoire and genetics of zebrafish allows the researchers to conclude that social recognition and social motivation can function as independent entities, this conclusion amplifies the need to study neural mechanisms regulating specific social behaviors, rather than the more generalized “social brain.”

Not all specific social behaviors, including social behaviors characteristic of humans, can be studied in standard laboratory species. The previous volume of the special issue highlighted the importance of choosing appropriate species for such studies by discussing the power of experiments in socially monogamous prairie voles for understanding the neurobiology of pair-bonding.5, 6 The research paper by Mederos, Duarte and colleagues in the current issue initiates the development of a novel fish model that could enhance our understanding of pair-bonding.7 The paper is based on the premise of frequently observed social and genetic monogamy across several related species of seahorses,8-10 which suggests that they are capable of pair-bonding. In their paper, the authors investigate color changes as a form of social communication in lined seahorses (Hippocampus erectus). They demonstrate that the color change (increase in brightness) occurs more frequently, especially in male fish, when they are paired with their pair mate, than when they are paired with an opposite-sex stranger. In a subsequent preliminary whole brain RNA sequencing analysis authors detect a substantial overlap in pairing-induced changes in gene expression between male seahorses and prairie voles. Remarkably, oxytocin receptor is among upregulated genes in paired female seahorses, in agreement with its well-known importance in pair-bonding in other species.

Animal species adopt different modes of social communication depending on evolutionary constraints. Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) are of great importance for social communication in rodents. Previous research demonstrated that communication via USVs depends on age, sex and can also differ between mouse strains.11 However, the detailed understanding of differences in such communication and its relationship to underlying behavioral situation is missing. In their research paper, Caruso et al.12 provide an extensive analysis of vocal repertoire across several early postnatal ages and in adults of three mouse strains: the commonly used inbred C57BL/6N, the highly social inbred FVB and the outbred CD-1. Moreover, the USVs were analyzed across three behavioral situations: social separation in mouse pups, adult male–female encounters and adult female–female encounters. In parallel, the authors assess the behavioral repertoire of adult mice during the interactions. While the USV repertoire of mice changes from early postnatal to adult age, strain- and social context-dependent differences are ever present in all assessments of vocalizations.

The behavioral repertoire of animals can not only differ between different ages, genotypes and social context, but can be even different between cagemate rodents of the same strain, sex and age. Animals living in groups usually organize themselves into social hierarchies. Behavioral repertoires, as well as general well-being, are different between socially-dominant and subordinate individuals within these groups.13 Park and colleagues in their research paper address an intriguing question whether social rank affects empathy-like behavior in mice.14 The authors determine social ranks in male C57BL/6J mice using the well-established tube test for social ranking and then evaluate these mice for their ability to develop empathy-like observational fear learning (OFL). In OFL, “observer” mice acquire fear from “demonstrator” mice undergoing fear conditioning. The authors demonstrate that intermediately ranking observers are better at acquiring fear from dominant than from subordinate demonstrators. In the same direction of effect, subordinate observers are better than dominant cagemates at acquiring fear from intermediately ranking demonstrators. Moreover, the OFL is dependent on the steepness of the social hierarchy. These findings are in agreement with a previous study showing that the subordinate rank in rats enhances social fear transmission,15 suggesting generality of this effect across species. However, the previous studies were not able to discern whether this increased fear is due to familiarity of the observer with the social rank of the demonstrator or due to some intrinsic quality of dominant demonstrator. Park and colleagues rule out the second possibility by showing that social ranking does not affect OFL from unfamiliar mice. Their study definitively determines higher empathy-like responses in more subordinate versus more dominant cagemates, at least in male mice. While not the main focus of the study, this article also shows that both single-housing and crowding strongly inhibit empathy-like OFL in mice calling to attention the importance of housing conditions for assessing social behaviors.

The latter observation is well in line with the next article in this special issue. The research paper by Winiarsky, Kondrakiewicz and colleagues compares effects of housing, habituation and testing conditions in males of commonly used C57BL/6J mice versus BTBR T+ Iptr3tf/J mice (BTBR) mice.16 The BTBR mice are a recognized idiopathic model of aspects of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and as such demonstrate deficits in social behaviors.17, 18 In the first series of experiments, the authors test these mice in the 3-chamber social affiliation test under several low and high stress conditions. These conditions range from testing non-habituated mice under relatively bright light conditions to habituated to handing and transportation mice, housed in enriched environment and tested in dim light. Behavior of the mice changes substantially depending on these conditions, at time obscuring the ability to detect strain differences. In the second series of experiment, the authors use an automated Eco-HAB system,19 which allows long-lasting evaluations of group-housed mice without disturbing them, to assess differences in social behaviors between these strains of mice. Among the identified striking differences, C57BL/6J mice demonstrate stability of social structure, whereas only limited subgroups of interactive BTBR mice engage in sporadic social interactions in these settings. Authors discuss the pros and cons for each of the two approaches for evaluating differences in the Systems for Social Processes Domain between mouse strains and highlight how differences in experimental conditions can clearly contribute to varied results.

The special issue is concluded with two important reviews on neurodevelopmental disorders and their animal models. The review by Benedetti and colleagues takes on one genetic mechanism with high penetrance for such disorders.20 Namely, authors screened over 750 publications and analyzed 58 studies in humans and 16 studies in mice that focused on behavioral characterization of subjects with copy number variations (CNVs) in DNA segments 16p11.2 and 22q11.2. CNVs on these genetic loci are associated with substantially increased risk for ASD, schizophrenia and attention deficit disorder.21, 22 Mutant mice with deletions of appropriate DNA loci have been developed. These mouse mutants provide models with excellent construct validity for the corresponding CNVs in humans. Authors review deficits in the several social domains in subjects with CMVs (involving social interaction, social communication, social memory and social cognition) and criteria for their identification. They report on several matching changes in tests of sociability, social interaction and social memory and communication (vis USVs) in the mutant mice. These commonalities in observations between human and mouse studies are promising for better understanding of associated disorders. However, on one hand, the number of behavioral tests assessed mice with relevant CNVs are sparse, compared with the vast array of murine social behavioral tests available. On the other hand, even the performed tests do not align well with the psychiatric diagnostic criteria used in humans. While highlighting the deficiencies, Benedetti and colleagues discuss several exciting avenues for further testing in rodent models (for example, assessing recognition of visual cues, parental influences, immunological mechanisms and effects of microbiome).

While an increase in studies using relevant tests on social behaviors would benefit understanding of deficits in social behaviors associated with CNVs, the final paper in this issue questions how the animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders should be optimally utilized. Silverman and colleagues report on recommendations of the workshop held by the Autism Science Foundation with the goal of improving utilization of such models.23 The resulting review collected 148 and analyzed 69 articles on ASD mouse models. The analysis reveals inconsistent reporting patterns in behavioral performance, experimental design and insufficient alignment of behavioral testing with diagnostic criteria of ASD. The authors emphasize the need to focus on construct validity of models, rather than on their face validity, as well as the importance of not underestimating the complexity of behavior in animal models. Silverman and colleagues acknowledge the challenges of translating between mouse and human behaviors, the complication of results due to use of various genetic backgrounds, use of different methodologies across labs, problems with inter-laboratory reproducibility, often insufficiently clear or incorrectly applied statistical analyses, lack of appreciation for temporal trajectories of phenotypes, the need to go beyond mice to represent the full repertoire of complex behaviors and the need to characterize models of neurodevelopmental disorders across multiple behavioral domains. The authors recommend that researchers assess not only social behavior in their models but measure cognitive and motor abilities, assess their models across a spectrum of comorbidities, keep in mind the role of olfaction in rodent social interactions, control for underlying motivation of subjects in their studies, all the while sticking to meticulous reporting of experimental design, detailed methods, materials, results and statistical analyses.

We conclude this series of special issues on social behaviors, autism and neurodevelopmental disorders by emphasizing that this exciting research field exhibits much motivation to go beyond superficial observations of simplistic phenotypes. The front cover image of this special issue, by Dr. Yangmiao Zhang, illustrates that participants in social interactions come with different personal experiences and backgrounds. These experiences guide them to exhibit various behaviors, including social bonding. May the complexity of social behaviors serve as a common challenge to bond neuroscientists in the pursuit of intricate underlying mechanisms.

从基本的社会神经生物学到更好地理解神经发育障碍
本卷总结了我们的基因,大脑和行为特刊三部分系列。前两卷从自闭症和神经发育障碍的研究开始,得出的结论是,如果不了解先天的社会行为,就不可能充分理解它们,并继续研究几种动物的这种先天行为的机制。1,2当前的特刊继续分析先天社会行为,然后将我们带回到神经发育障碍动物模型的研究,希望对社会神经生物学的复杂性有更大的理解。Goncalves、Kareklas和他们的同事在他们的研究论文中优雅地解决了一个关于进化的基本问题:构成社会行为的动机和认知成分是独立进化的,还是通过选择性压力来增加社会性为了解决这个问题,作者在四种行为测试中对雄性和雌性斑马鱼(Danio rerio)进行了表型表征:鱼群偏好、同向识别、物体识别和开放区域。这种特征将行为分为三个主要因素:动机、认知和焦虑。重要的是,该分析中的社会倾向模块与对象和社会探索一致,而与对象歧视和社会歧视分开聚类,两者一致。这些数据有力地支持了社会认知和社会动机并没有作为一种共同的表型一起进化,而是分别选择了一般的认知和动机机制的假设。单核苷酸多态性(snp)和行为模块分析进一步支持了这一结论。遗传分析表明,与社会行为相关的候选基因的snp在统计学上与动机模块相关,而与认知模块无关。这一发现与最近的报道一致,即缺乏功能性催产素受体的斑马鱼缺乏社会和物体识别,但具有完整的社会动机虽然斑马鱼的行为特征和遗传学使研究人员得出结论,社会认知和社会动机可以作为独立的实体发挥作用,但这一结论放大了研究调节特定社会行为的神经机制的必要性,而不是更广义的“社会大脑”。并不是所有特定的社会行为,包括人类的社会行为特征,都可以在标准的实验室物种中进行研究。特刊的前一卷强调了选择合适的物种进行这类研究的重要性,讨论了社会一夫一妻制草原田鼠实验的力量,以理解成对结合的神经生物学。Mederos, Duarte及其同事在本期杂志上发表的研究论文开创了一种新的鱼类模型,可以增强我们对成对结合的理解这篇论文的前提是,在几个相关的海马物种中,经常观察到社会和遗传上的一夫一妻制,这表明它们能够结成伴侣。在他们的论文中,作者研究了条纹海马(海马体直立人)的一种社会交流形式——颜色变化。他们证明了颜色变化(亮度增加)发生得更频繁,尤其是在雄鱼身上,当它们与配偶配对时,比与异性陌生人配对时更频繁。在随后的初步全脑RNA测序分析中,作者发现雄性海马和草原田鼠在配对诱导的基因表达变化中存在大量重叠。值得注意的是,催产素受体在配对雌性海马中是上调的基因之一,这与它在其他物种的配对中众所周知的重要性是一致的。根据进化的限制,动物物种采用不同的社会交流模式。超声发声在啮齿类动物的社会交流中具有重要意义。先前的研究表明,通过usv进行交流取决于年龄、性别,而且在不同的小鼠品系之间也会有所不同然而,对这种交流的差异及其与潜在行为情境的关系的详细了解尚不清楚。在他们的研究论文中,Caruso等人12对三种小鼠品系(常用的近交系C57BL/6N,高度社会化的近交系FVB和近交系CD-1)的几个早期出生年龄和成年小鼠的声乐曲目进行了广泛的分析。此外,还分析了三种行为情况下的usv:幼鼠社会分离、成年雄性雌性相遇和成年雌性雌性相遇。同时,作者评估了成年小鼠在互动过程中的行为。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
ACS Applied Bio Materials
ACS Applied Bio Materials Chemistry-Chemistry (all)
CiteScore
9.40
自引率
2.10%
发文量
464
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信