管理指数级增长的孟德尔随机化研究。

IF 5.2 1区 医学 Q1 PSYCHIATRY
Addiction Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI:10.1111/add.16627
Marcus R. Munafò, Jamie Brown, Marita Hefler, George Davey Smith
{"title":"管理指数级增长的孟德尔随机化研究。","authors":"Marcus R. Munafò,&nbsp;Jamie Brown,&nbsp;Marita Hefler,&nbsp;George Davey Smith","doi":"10.1111/add.16627","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Much of the research we publish relates to questions of cause and effect. In an ideal world, we would subject these questions to experimentation, randomizing study participants to different conditions. However, in many cases—particularly in the context of addiction—such randomization is simply not possible. We cannot randomize tobacco-naïve children to use e-cigarettes, for example, to determine whether or not vaping acts as a ‘gateway’ to subsequent smoking. In these cases, we have to rely on observational methods, and these suffer from well described problems of confounding, including reverse causality.</p><p>Several methods exist for strengthening causal inference in such cases, from the use of prospective data and statistical adjustment for confounding through to propensity score matching and the use of natural experiments. One method, in particular, has experienced exponential growth recently—Mendelian randomization (MR) [<span>1, 2</span>]. This approach uses genetic variants as proxies for an exposure of interest, effectively as a form of instrumental variable analysis. If relevant assumptions hold, this should protect against confounding, including reverse causality, due to the random allocation of genotype at meiosis and the fact that environmental exposures cannot directly alter germline DNA sequence [<span>3</span>].</p><p>As summary data from a vast range of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have become widely and freely available, it has become possible to run every permutation of the vast number of exposure-outcome relationships that exist using MR methods. This, in principle, is a good thing. Although MR is not without its limitations (and critics), it is a potentially powerful tool that has provided important evidence—for example, suggesting a causal effect of cigarette smoking on some adverse mental health outcomes [<span>4</span>]. However, with the advent of platforms such as MR Base (https://www.mrbase.org), all bivariate relationships tractable via MR using summary GWAS data can be considered to have been conducted [<span>5</span>].</p><p>Unfortunately, GWAS with ever-larger samples have enabled us to detect genetic variants associated with an exposure of interest that are <i>also</i> associated with a range of other exposures (and not <i>via</i> the exposure of interest), effectively reintroducing confounding when using MR. For example, genetic variants associated with smoking initiation are also associated with behavioural outcomes in young children, at an age before any exposure to smoking, suggesting that these variants may not be uniquely capturing smoking initiation, but instead some broad risk-taking phenotype [<span>6</span>].</p><p>In other words, while the exposure of interest may be smoking initiation, the primary phenotype most proximal to the genetic proxies used may be risk taking. This means that using these variants as a proxy for smoking initiation may introduce genetic confounding. Dynastic effects may also be operating, whereby offspring genetic variants become associated with particular environmental exposures due to parental genotypes influencing these exposures and (of course) offspring genotype. This is likely to be a particular issue in the context of substance use.</p><p>Taken together, this means that using MR in the context of complex behavioural exposures requires careful thought—the use of negative controls to exclude alternative pathways to the outcome of interest and ideally triangulation of evidence using multiple study designs, combined with a detailed understanding of the plausible biological pathways where possible [<span>7</span>].</p><p>Despite this, we are unfortunately seeing an ever-increasing number of MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data, and lack negative controls or evidence from other study designs and methodologies to strengthen inference. These often investigate causal pathways that are already known (e.g. whether smoking causes coronary arterial disease) or exposures that simply do not lend themselves to genetic instrumentation within an MR framework (e.g. skipping breakfast). Ultimately, these studies either do not advance knowledge (because the answer is already known) or offer little more than a conventional observational study. Indeed, they may offer <i>less</i>, and in fact have negative utility, in that they come packaged with causal claims, in a way that conventional observational studies typically do not. In this way, they may actually serve to degrade knowledge.</p><p>What is driving this increase? Ultimately, it is down to current incentive structures that reward publication over knowledge.</p><p>An indicator of this is that there are now relatively few studies applying MR methods that report null results. This is ironic. A key early application of MR was to establish whether widely reported conventional observational analyses were causal. For example, circulating C-reactive protein (CRP) is associated with coronary heart disease (CHD) and the development of novel therapeutics targeted CRP because of its presumed causal role; however, MR analyses established that CRP does <i>not</i> cause elevated CHD, with the likely reason for the observed association being that early stages of the atherosclerotic disease process increase CRP levels, as do many established causes of CHD such as cigarette smoking and elevated adiposity [<span>8, 9</span>]. Another key null early MR finding related to high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, which was widely considered to reduce CHD risk; MR and RCTs concurred in demonstrating the lack of benefit of higher HDL [<span>10</span>]. Such null results are critical in correcting erroneous findings in observational epidemiology.</p><p>There are other ways to use MR that genuinely add to the sum of human knowledge. For example, Khouja and colleagues [<span>11</span>] used multivariable MR to attempt to dissect the effects of nicotine and non-nicotine constituents of tobacco smoke on outcomes known to be caused by smoking. And Davies and colleagues [<span>12</span>] triangulated evidence from MR and the natural experiment of the raising of the school leaving age in the United Kingdom to understand the causal effects of educational attainment on smoking initiation.</p><p>MR analyses have been already used to good effect in a range of areas relevant to addictive behaviours. For example, in conventional analyses, ‘moderate’ alcohol intake is associated with reduced cardiovascular risk, when compared with abstinence or heavier drinking. These findings, repeatedly reported over several decades, achieved widespread recognition.</p><p>However, using genetic variants that strongly influence alcohol consumption, together with the natural experiment created by few women drinking in east Asian countries, genotype-predicted alcohol intake was shown to be linearly related to blood pressure; critically, this was only observed in men—the lack of a relationship among women (who drank virtually no alcohol) indicated that the genetic variants did not have an effect on blood pressure except through their relationship with alcohol intake [<span>13</span>]. Using the same approach, alcohol was shown to have a continuous linear adverse effect on stroke risk [<span>14</span>]. As mentioned above, MR was also used to demonstrate that HDL cholesterol—that which was supposed to mediate the protective effect of alcohol on CHD—was not actually protective. Thus, the evidence from MR studies and that from RCTs triangulated to clarify one of the most controversial issues in cardiovascular epidemiology.</p><p>We therefore suggest that MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data to estimate the causal effect of X on Y should be given a low priority for publication unless they genuinely advance knowledge. This could be achieved by exploring complex exposures and/or outcomes (including testing for possible mediation via multivariable MR), incorporating negative controls and/or evidence from other study designs such as natural experiments, and collaborating with biologists to advance plausible biological mechanisms. MR studies should also conform to the reporting guidelines in the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology using Mendelian Randomization (STROBE-MR) guidelines [<span>15</span>].</p><p>We do not wish to be overly prescriptive, but ultimately if a study offers nothing more than the mechanical application of a statistical package to publicly available summary data, then it may not warrant the use of editorial and reviewer time, and journal space.</p><p><b>Marcus R. Munafò:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — original draft (lead); writing — review and editing (equal). <b>Jamie Brown:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal). <b>Marita Hefler:</b> Writing — review and editing (equal). <b>George Davey Smith:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal).</p><p>None.</p><p>MRM leads an MRC Programme that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/07). JB has received unrestricted funding to study smoking cessation from J&amp;J and Pfizer, who manufacture medically licensed smoking cessation pharmacotherapies. MH has no competing interests to declare. GDS coauthored the first extended exposition of Mendelian randomization, and therefore has considerable intellectual investment in the approach and has received funding for MR studies over many years. He directs an MRC Unit that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/01).</p>","PeriodicalId":109,"journal":{"name":"Addiction","volume":"119 11","pages":"1861-1863"},"PeriodicalIF":5.2000,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/add.16627","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Managing the exponential growth of Mendelian randomization studies\",\"authors\":\"Marcus R. Munafò,&nbsp;Jamie Brown,&nbsp;Marita Hefler,&nbsp;George Davey Smith\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/add.16627\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Much of the research we publish relates to questions of cause and effect. In an ideal world, we would subject these questions to experimentation, randomizing study participants to different conditions. However, in many cases—particularly in the context of addiction—such randomization is simply not possible. We cannot randomize tobacco-naïve children to use e-cigarettes, for example, to determine whether or not vaping acts as a ‘gateway’ to subsequent smoking. In these cases, we have to rely on observational methods, and these suffer from well described problems of confounding, including reverse causality.</p><p>Several methods exist for strengthening causal inference in such cases, from the use of prospective data and statistical adjustment for confounding through to propensity score matching and the use of natural experiments. One method, in particular, has experienced exponential growth recently—Mendelian randomization (MR) [<span>1, 2</span>]. This approach uses genetic variants as proxies for an exposure of interest, effectively as a form of instrumental variable analysis. If relevant assumptions hold, this should protect against confounding, including reverse causality, due to the random allocation of genotype at meiosis and the fact that environmental exposures cannot directly alter germline DNA sequence [<span>3</span>].</p><p>As summary data from a vast range of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have become widely and freely available, it has become possible to run every permutation of the vast number of exposure-outcome relationships that exist using MR methods. This, in principle, is a good thing. Although MR is not without its limitations (and critics), it is a potentially powerful tool that has provided important evidence—for example, suggesting a causal effect of cigarette smoking on some adverse mental health outcomes [<span>4</span>]. However, with the advent of platforms such as MR Base (https://www.mrbase.org), all bivariate relationships tractable via MR using summary GWAS data can be considered to have been conducted [<span>5</span>].</p><p>Unfortunately, GWAS with ever-larger samples have enabled us to detect genetic variants associated with an exposure of interest that are <i>also</i> associated with a range of other exposures (and not <i>via</i> the exposure of interest), effectively reintroducing confounding when using MR. For example, genetic variants associated with smoking initiation are also associated with behavioural outcomes in young children, at an age before any exposure to smoking, suggesting that these variants may not be uniquely capturing smoking initiation, but instead some broad risk-taking phenotype [<span>6</span>].</p><p>In other words, while the exposure of interest may be smoking initiation, the primary phenotype most proximal to the genetic proxies used may be risk taking. This means that using these variants as a proxy for smoking initiation may introduce genetic confounding. Dynastic effects may also be operating, whereby offspring genetic variants become associated with particular environmental exposures due to parental genotypes influencing these exposures and (of course) offspring genotype. This is likely to be a particular issue in the context of substance use.</p><p>Taken together, this means that using MR in the context of complex behavioural exposures requires careful thought—the use of negative controls to exclude alternative pathways to the outcome of interest and ideally triangulation of evidence using multiple study designs, combined with a detailed understanding of the plausible biological pathways where possible [<span>7</span>].</p><p>Despite this, we are unfortunately seeing an ever-increasing number of MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data, and lack negative controls or evidence from other study designs and methodologies to strengthen inference. These often investigate causal pathways that are already known (e.g. whether smoking causes coronary arterial disease) or exposures that simply do not lend themselves to genetic instrumentation within an MR framework (e.g. skipping breakfast). Ultimately, these studies either do not advance knowledge (because the answer is already known) or offer little more than a conventional observational study. Indeed, they may offer <i>less</i>, and in fact have negative utility, in that they come packaged with causal claims, in a way that conventional observational studies typically do not. In this way, they may actually serve to degrade knowledge.</p><p>What is driving this increase? Ultimately, it is down to current incentive structures that reward publication over knowledge.</p><p>An indicator of this is that there are now relatively few studies applying MR methods that report null results. This is ironic. A key early application of MR was to establish whether widely reported conventional observational analyses were causal. For example, circulating C-reactive protein (CRP) is associated with coronary heart disease (CHD) and the development of novel therapeutics targeted CRP because of its presumed causal role; however, MR analyses established that CRP does <i>not</i> cause elevated CHD, with the likely reason for the observed association being that early stages of the atherosclerotic disease process increase CRP levels, as do many established causes of CHD such as cigarette smoking and elevated adiposity [<span>8, 9</span>]. Another key null early MR finding related to high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, which was widely considered to reduce CHD risk; MR and RCTs concurred in demonstrating the lack of benefit of higher HDL [<span>10</span>]. Such null results are critical in correcting erroneous findings in observational epidemiology.</p><p>There are other ways to use MR that genuinely add to the sum of human knowledge. For example, Khouja and colleagues [<span>11</span>] used multivariable MR to attempt to dissect the effects of nicotine and non-nicotine constituents of tobacco smoke on outcomes known to be caused by smoking. And Davies and colleagues [<span>12</span>] triangulated evidence from MR and the natural experiment of the raising of the school leaving age in the United Kingdom to understand the causal effects of educational attainment on smoking initiation.</p><p>MR analyses have been already used to good effect in a range of areas relevant to addictive behaviours. For example, in conventional analyses, ‘moderate’ alcohol intake is associated with reduced cardiovascular risk, when compared with abstinence or heavier drinking. These findings, repeatedly reported over several decades, achieved widespread recognition.</p><p>However, using genetic variants that strongly influence alcohol consumption, together with the natural experiment created by few women drinking in east Asian countries, genotype-predicted alcohol intake was shown to be linearly related to blood pressure; critically, this was only observed in men—the lack of a relationship among women (who drank virtually no alcohol) indicated that the genetic variants did not have an effect on blood pressure except through their relationship with alcohol intake [<span>13</span>]. Using the same approach, alcohol was shown to have a continuous linear adverse effect on stroke risk [<span>14</span>]. As mentioned above, MR was also used to demonstrate that HDL cholesterol—that which was supposed to mediate the protective effect of alcohol on CHD—was not actually protective. Thus, the evidence from MR studies and that from RCTs triangulated to clarify one of the most controversial issues in cardiovascular epidemiology.</p><p>We therefore suggest that MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data to estimate the causal effect of X on Y should be given a low priority for publication unless they genuinely advance knowledge. This could be achieved by exploring complex exposures and/or outcomes (including testing for possible mediation via multivariable MR), incorporating negative controls and/or evidence from other study designs such as natural experiments, and collaborating with biologists to advance plausible biological mechanisms. MR studies should also conform to the reporting guidelines in the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology using Mendelian Randomization (STROBE-MR) guidelines [<span>15</span>].</p><p>We do not wish to be overly prescriptive, but ultimately if a study offers nothing more than the mechanical application of a statistical package to publicly available summary data, then it may not warrant the use of editorial and reviewer time, and journal space.</p><p><b>Marcus R. Munafò:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — original draft (lead); writing — review and editing (equal). <b>Jamie Brown:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal). <b>Marita Hefler:</b> Writing — review and editing (equal). <b>George Davey Smith:</b> Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal).</p><p>None.</p><p>MRM leads an MRC Programme that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/07). JB has received unrestricted funding to study smoking cessation from J&amp;J and Pfizer, who manufacture medically licensed smoking cessation pharmacotherapies. MH has no competing interests to declare. GDS coauthored the first extended exposition of Mendelian randomization, and therefore has considerable intellectual investment in the approach and has received funding for MR studies over many years. He directs an MRC Unit that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/01).</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":109,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Addiction\",\"volume\":\"119 11\",\"pages\":\"1861-1863\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":5.2000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-09-23\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/add.16627\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Addiction\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/add.16627\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"医学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHIATRY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Addiction","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/add.16627","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

我们发表的许多研究都与因果问题有关。在理想的世界里,我们会对这些问题进行实验,将研究参与者随机分配到不同的条件下。然而,在很多情况下,尤其是在成瘾的情况下,这种随机化是根本不可能的。例如,我们无法随机安排对烟草不敏感的儿童使用电子烟,以确定吸食电子烟是否会成为以后吸烟的 "途径"。在这种情况下,我们不得不依赖观察方法,而这些方法存在着众所周知的混杂问题,包括反向因果关系。在这种情况下,有几种方法可以加强因果推断,从使用前瞻性数据和对混杂因素进行统计调整,到倾向得分匹配和使用自然实验。其中一种方法--孟德尔随机化(Mendelian randomization,MR)[1, 2]--尤其在最近经历了指数式增长。这种方法使用基因变异作为相关暴露的替代物,实际上是一种工具变量分析。由于减数分裂时基因型的随机分配以及环境暴露不能直接改变种系 DNA 序列的事实,如果相关假设成立,这种方法应能防止混杂,包括反向因果关系[3]。随着大量全基因组关联研究(GWAS)的汇总数据被广泛免费提供,使用 MR 方法对大量存在的暴露-结果关系的每一种变异进行分析已成为可能。这原则上是件好事。虽然 MR 并非没有局限性(和批评),但它是一种潜在的强大工具,提供了重要的证据--例如,表明吸烟对某些不良心理健康结果具有因果效应[4]。然而,随着 MR Base(https://www.mrbase.org)等平台的出现,使用 GWAS 数据摘要通过 MR 可得出的所有二元关系都可以被认为已经得到了证实[5]。不幸的是,GWAS 的样本越来越大,使我们能够检测到与所关注的暴露相关的基因变异,而这些变异也与一系列其他暴露相关(而不是通过所关注的暴露),这实际上在使用 MR 时重新引入了混杂因素。例如,与开始吸烟有关的基因变异也与幼儿的行为结果有关,而幼儿的年龄还没有接触过吸烟,这表明这些变异可能并不是唯一捕捉到开始吸烟的基因变异,而是某种广泛的冒险表型[6]。换句话说,虽然感兴趣的暴露可能是开始吸烟,但与所用基因替代物最接近的主要表型可能是冒险。这意味着使用这些变异作为开始吸烟的替代物可能会带来遗传混杂。此外,还可能存在动态效应,即由于父母的基因型影响了特定的环境暴露(当然还有后代的基因型),后代的基因变异与这些环境暴露相关联。总之,这意味着在复杂的行为暴露背景下使用 MR 需要深思熟虑--使用阴性对照排除导致相关结果的替代途径,最好使用多种研究设计对证据进行三角测量,并在可能的情况下结合对合理生物途径的详细了解[7]。这些研究往往调查的是已知的因果途径(如吸烟是否会导致冠状动脉疾病),或者是根本不适合在 MR 框架内进行基因检测的暴露(如不吃早餐)。归根结底,这些研究要么不能增进知识(因为答案已经揭晓),要么只能提供比传统观察研究更多的信息。事实上,这些研究提供的信息可能更少,而且实际上具有负面作用,因为它们带有因果关系的主张,而传统的观察性研究通常不带因果关系的主张。是什么原因导致了这种增长?归根结底,这是由于当前的激励结构奖励发表而非知识。一个指标是,目前应用磁共振方法进行的研究中,报告结果为空的相对较少。这具有讽刺意味。MR 早期的一个重要应用是确定广泛报道的常规观察分析是否具有因果关系。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Managing the exponential growth of Mendelian randomization studies

Much of the research we publish relates to questions of cause and effect. In an ideal world, we would subject these questions to experimentation, randomizing study participants to different conditions. However, in many cases—particularly in the context of addiction—such randomization is simply not possible. We cannot randomize tobacco-naïve children to use e-cigarettes, for example, to determine whether or not vaping acts as a ‘gateway’ to subsequent smoking. In these cases, we have to rely on observational methods, and these suffer from well described problems of confounding, including reverse causality.

Several methods exist for strengthening causal inference in such cases, from the use of prospective data and statistical adjustment for confounding through to propensity score matching and the use of natural experiments. One method, in particular, has experienced exponential growth recently—Mendelian randomization (MR) [1, 2]. This approach uses genetic variants as proxies for an exposure of interest, effectively as a form of instrumental variable analysis. If relevant assumptions hold, this should protect against confounding, including reverse causality, due to the random allocation of genotype at meiosis and the fact that environmental exposures cannot directly alter germline DNA sequence [3].

As summary data from a vast range of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have become widely and freely available, it has become possible to run every permutation of the vast number of exposure-outcome relationships that exist using MR methods. This, in principle, is a good thing. Although MR is not without its limitations (and critics), it is a potentially powerful tool that has provided important evidence—for example, suggesting a causal effect of cigarette smoking on some adverse mental health outcomes [4]. However, with the advent of platforms such as MR Base (https://www.mrbase.org), all bivariate relationships tractable via MR using summary GWAS data can be considered to have been conducted [5].

Unfortunately, GWAS with ever-larger samples have enabled us to detect genetic variants associated with an exposure of interest that are also associated with a range of other exposures (and not via the exposure of interest), effectively reintroducing confounding when using MR. For example, genetic variants associated with smoking initiation are also associated with behavioural outcomes in young children, at an age before any exposure to smoking, suggesting that these variants may not be uniquely capturing smoking initiation, but instead some broad risk-taking phenotype [6].

In other words, while the exposure of interest may be smoking initiation, the primary phenotype most proximal to the genetic proxies used may be risk taking. This means that using these variants as a proxy for smoking initiation may introduce genetic confounding. Dynastic effects may also be operating, whereby offspring genetic variants become associated with particular environmental exposures due to parental genotypes influencing these exposures and (of course) offspring genotype. This is likely to be a particular issue in the context of substance use.

Taken together, this means that using MR in the context of complex behavioural exposures requires careful thought—the use of negative controls to exclude alternative pathways to the outcome of interest and ideally triangulation of evidence using multiple study designs, combined with a detailed understanding of the plausible biological pathways where possible [7].

Despite this, we are unfortunately seeing an ever-increasing number of MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data, and lack negative controls or evidence from other study designs and methodologies to strengthen inference. These often investigate causal pathways that are already known (e.g. whether smoking causes coronary arterial disease) or exposures that simply do not lend themselves to genetic instrumentation within an MR framework (e.g. skipping breakfast). Ultimately, these studies either do not advance knowledge (because the answer is already known) or offer little more than a conventional observational study. Indeed, they may offer less, and in fact have negative utility, in that they come packaged with causal claims, in a way that conventional observational studies typically do not. In this way, they may actually serve to degrade knowledge.

What is driving this increase? Ultimately, it is down to current incentive structures that reward publication over knowledge.

An indicator of this is that there are now relatively few studies applying MR methods that report null results. This is ironic. A key early application of MR was to establish whether widely reported conventional observational analyses were causal. For example, circulating C-reactive protein (CRP) is associated with coronary heart disease (CHD) and the development of novel therapeutics targeted CRP because of its presumed causal role; however, MR analyses established that CRP does not cause elevated CHD, with the likely reason for the observed association being that early stages of the atherosclerotic disease process increase CRP levels, as do many established causes of CHD such as cigarette smoking and elevated adiposity [8, 9]. Another key null early MR finding related to high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, which was widely considered to reduce CHD risk; MR and RCTs concurred in demonstrating the lack of benefit of higher HDL [10]. Such null results are critical in correcting erroneous findings in observational epidemiology.

There are other ways to use MR that genuinely add to the sum of human knowledge. For example, Khouja and colleagues [11] used multivariable MR to attempt to dissect the effects of nicotine and non-nicotine constituents of tobacco smoke on outcomes known to be caused by smoking. And Davies and colleagues [12] triangulated evidence from MR and the natural experiment of the raising of the school leaving age in the United Kingdom to understand the causal effects of educational attainment on smoking initiation.

MR analyses have been already used to good effect in a range of areas relevant to addictive behaviours. For example, in conventional analyses, ‘moderate’ alcohol intake is associated with reduced cardiovascular risk, when compared with abstinence or heavier drinking. These findings, repeatedly reported over several decades, achieved widespread recognition.

However, using genetic variants that strongly influence alcohol consumption, together with the natural experiment created by few women drinking in east Asian countries, genotype-predicted alcohol intake was shown to be linearly related to blood pressure; critically, this was only observed in men—the lack of a relationship among women (who drank virtually no alcohol) indicated that the genetic variants did not have an effect on blood pressure except through their relationship with alcohol intake [13]. Using the same approach, alcohol was shown to have a continuous linear adverse effect on stroke risk [14]. As mentioned above, MR was also used to demonstrate that HDL cholesterol—that which was supposed to mediate the protective effect of alcohol on CHD—was not actually protective. Thus, the evidence from MR studies and that from RCTs triangulated to clarify one of the most controversial issues in cardiovascular epidemiology.

We therefore suggest that MR studies that simply use summary GWAS data to estimate the causal effect of X on Y should be given a low priority for publication unless they genuinely advance knowledge. This could be achieved by exploring complex exposures and/or outcomes (including testing for possible mediation via multivariable MR), incorporating negative controls and/or evidence from other study designs such as natural experiments, and collaborating with biologists to advance plausible biological mechanisms. MR studies should also conform to the reporting guidelines in the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology using Mendelian Randomization (STROBE-MR) guidelines [15].

We do not wish to be overly prescriptive, but ultimately if a study offers nothing more than the mechanical application of a statistical package to publicly available summary data, then it may not warrant the use of editorial and reviewer time, and journal space.

Marcus R. Munafò: Conceptualization (equal); writing — original draft (lead); writing — review and editing (equal). Jamie Brown: Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal). Marita Hefler: Writing — review and editing (equal). George Davey Smith: Conceptualization (equal); writing — review and editing (equal).

None.

MRM leads an MRC Programme that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/07). JB has received unrestricted funding to study smoking cessation from J&J and Pfizer, who manufacture medically licensed smoking cessation pharmacotherapies. MH has no competing interests to declare. GDS coauthored the first extended exposition of Mendelian randomization, and therefore has considerable intellectual investment in the approach and has received funding for MR studies over many years. He directs an MRC Unit that conducts a substantial amount of MR research (MC_UU_00032/01).

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Addiction
Addiction 医学-精神病学
CiteScore
10.80
自引率
6.70%
发文量
319
审稿时长
3 months
期刊介绍: Addiction publishes peer-reviewed research reports on pharmacological and behavioural addictions, bringing together research conducted within many different disciplines. Its goal is to serve international and interdisciplinary scientific and clinical communication, to strengthen links between science and policy, and to stimulate and enhance the quality of debate. We seek submissions that are not only technically competent but are also original and contain information or ideas of fresh interest to our international readership. We seek to serve low- and middle-income (LAMI) countries as well as more economically developed countries. Addiction’s scope spans human experimental, epidemiological, social science, historical, clinical and policy research relating to addiction, primarily but not exclusively in the areas of psychoactive substance use and/or gambling. In addition to original research, the journal features editorials, commentaries, reviews, letters, and book reviews.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信