拍打翅膀:蜜蜂对蚂蚁的一种防御行为。

IF 4.4 2区 环境科学与生态学 Q1 ECOLOGY
Ecology Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI:10.1002/ecy.4372
Yugo Seko, Kiyohito Morii, Yoshiko Sakamoto
{"title":"拍打翅膀:蜜蜂对蚂蚁的一种防御行为。","authors":"Yugo Seko,&nbsp;Kiyohito Morii,&nbsp;Yoshiko Sakamoto","doi":"10.1002/ecy.4372","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Eusocial organisms are group living, and their nests, which are inhabited by numerous eggs, larvae, and adults, are attractive feeding grounds for predators and parasites. To effectively defend the nest against such natural enemies, eusocial organisms have evolved specific defensive behaviors. For example, honey bees (<i>Apis</i> spp.) counter attacks from hornets, an important predator, by the use of shimmering waves (Tan et al., <span>2012</span>) and defensive balling (Ono et al., <span>1995</span>); they also use grooming and other hygienic behaviors to suppress the spread of parasitic varroa mites (Harbo &amp; Harris, <span>2005</span>). Ants are also important consumers of honey, broods, and worker bees in beehives (Kern, <span>2017</span>), and it has been reported that some honey bee species inhibit the entry of ants into the hive through fan-blowing, a defensive behavior in which bees use wind pressure from fanning their wings to keep ants away from the hive entrance (Spangler &amp; Taer, <span>1970</span>). Because this behavior inhibits ant ingress, while avoiding direct contact between ants and the bees, it is thought to reduce the risk of injury or death of the defending bees (Seeley et al., <span>1982</span>). However, the Japanese honey bee (<i>Apis cerana japonica</i>), the Japanese subspecies of the eastern honey bee (<i>A. cerana</i>), repels ants by placing its wings directly in contact with intruding ants and flicking them away. Although this behavior has been briefly described in anecdotal accounts (Fujiwara et al., <span>2015</span>; Yoshida, <span>1997</span>), the detailed behavior sequence has remained unclear.</p><p>We filmed Japanese honey bee colonies being invaded by the Japanese pavement ant (<i>Tetramorium tsushimae</i>) with a high-speed camera (960 fps; DSC-RX10M4, Sony Group Corporation, Japan), and recorded honey bee defensive behaviors against the ants in detail. Our results show that the bees slap the ants through direct contact with their wings (Figure 1; Videos S1 and S2). Specifically, the worker bees executed the slap behavior by first tilting their bodies toward the ants, then flapping their wings while simultaneously turning their bodies. These observations demonstrate that the defensive behavior employed by Japanese honey bees against ants is clearly different from the noncontact fan-blowing behavior (Yang et al., <span>2010</span>) reported in other honey bees. We named this new defensive behavior “wing-slapping.”</p><p>In addition to the above observations, to identify whether wing-slapping behavior is only carried out against specific ant species and whether there are differences in effectiveness among ant species, we introduced ants to the vicinity of the hive entrance and determined the frequency of wing-slapping by Japanese honey bees. The observations were carried out in June and July 2023 on two colonies (A and B) of Japanese honey bees in Tsukuba City, Ibaraki Prefecture (36°02′54.1″ N, 140°06′54.8″ E). The Japanese honey bee colonies were raised in movable-frame hives with a platform of dimensions 50 × 260 mm at the hive entrance. Worker bees that remained on the entrance platform for more than 5 s were regarded as guard bees, and were marked with paint for identification. For the ant intruders, we selected three species from among the common Japanese ant species that are known to invade Japanese honey bee hives: the Japanese queenless ant <i>Pristomyrmex punctatus</i>, the Japanese pavement ant, and the Japanese wood ant <i>Formica japonica</i> (Miyano et al., <span>2001</span>; Okada, <span>1997</span>). These species were collected at the study site. Specimens were placed in a plastic cup with free access to the hive entrance. Then, the platform was filmed from directly above with a digital camera for 30 min. From the video, the behavior of marked guard bees was observed and recorded during periods when the distance between an ant and the center of the bee's head was 10 mm or less. In addition, the success rate of the wing-slaps was calculated. A wing-slap was deemed to be successful if it resulted in the targeted ant being flicked away.</p><p>Wing-slapping was the most commonly observed behavior performed by guard bees toward nearby ants (Figure 2a). However, western honey bees show distinct responses to different beetle species (Atkinson &amp; Ellis, <span>2011</span>), and more observations of Japanese honey bees responding to a variety of ant species are needed to understand whether their responses might also be species specific. In our study, whereas wing-slapping had a success rate of roughly one in two or three attempts in repelling intruding Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, the success rate against Japanese wood ants was relatively low (Figure 2b), suggesting the behavior might not be effective against all ant species. Given that Japanese wood ants have about twice the body length and move more quickly than the other species examined in the present study, it may have been difficult to successfully flick them away.</p><p>Wing-slapping behavior might have evolved among Japanese honey bees as a relatively low-energy, generic defense against the threat of ant incursion. Fan-blowing behavior is thought to function as a way for bees to avoid direct contact with predator ants (e.g., Asian weaver ants <i>Oecophylla smaragdina</i>; Seeley et al., <span>1982</span>). However, although Japanese ants, especially Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, will invade Japanese honey bee hives, they rarely bite and consume healthy workers in the hive (Okada, <span>1997</span>; Yoshida, <span>1997</span>; see Miyano et al., <span>2001</span> for an exception). Therefore, in Japan, where predator ants such as the Asian weaver ant are absent and the threat of direct predation by ants is low, noncontact defensive behaviors might not always be optimal. In other words, ants with low direct predation threat can be eliminated more efficiently (i.e., with lower energy cost) through direct contact rather than through generating wind pressure. To test this hypothesis, it will be necessary to investigate the aggression (predation ability) of ants and compare it against the defensive behaviors of bees in various areas. The behaviors employed to defend against ant incursion are yet to be identified for most honey bee species and subspecies. Such information, if collected in the future, would provide an interesting opportunity to examine the evolution of defensive behaviors among social insects through inter- and intraspecific comparison.</p><p>Anthropogenic range expansions can place honey bees in contact with predator species they have not evolved alongside, increasing the risk of predation. This risk has become a reality for some species, including the northern giant hornet (<i>Vespa mandarinia</i>) (Matsuura &amp; Sakagami, <span>1973</span>). The same logic would suggest that populations of the Japanese honey bee, which did not evolve alongside predatory ants, would suffer serious harm if exposed to nonnative ants that are capable of preying on bees. For example, big-headed ants prey upon healthy workers of western honey bees (Kern, <span>2017</span>; Yang et al., <span>2010</span>). Considering that big-headed ants are currently expanding their distribution in southern Japan (Yamane et al., <span>2019</span>), Japanese honey bees could come into frequent contact with them in the future. If Japanese honey bees respond to these nonnative predators with wing-slapping, which involves direct contact, they may be subjected to a higher risk of mortality than other honey bees that exhibit noncontact defensive responses. Invasive ants are a widespread problem around the world, and there is no doubt that new invasions will occur in the future. Therefore, the resulting harm to honey bees may become more severe over time.</p><p>Additionally, the defensive behaviors of honey bees against ants continue to raise numerous research questions, such as how bees detect ants, whether there are variations in the innate aptitude of individual honey bees for a given behavior, and whether the success rate of behavior increases with experience. Although ants are important consumers of bees and bee products (Kern, <span>2017</span>), defensive behaviors of bees against ants have received little attention, perhaps because these interactions are inconspicuous to human observers. Our study identifies a new defensive behavior, which we have named wing-slapping, and opens the door to further studies examining the evolution of defensive behaviors in social insects.</p><p>Yugo Seko and Kiyohito Morii collected and analyzed the data, took photographs and videos, and wrote the first draft. Yugo Seko, Kiyohito Morii, and Yoshiko Sakamoto designed the study and revised the manuscript.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>","PeriodicalId":11484,"journal":{"name":"Ecology","volume":"105 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4000,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ecy.4372","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Wing-slapping: A defensive behavior by honey bees against ants\",\"authors\":\"Yugo Seko,&nbsp;Kiyohito Morii,&nbsp;Yoshiko Sakamoto\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/ecy.4372\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Eusocial organisms are group living, and their nests, which are inhabited by numerous eggs, larvae, and adults, are attractive feeding grounds for predators and parasites. To effectively defend the nest against such natural enemies, eusocial organisms have evolved specific defensive behaviors. For example, honey bees (<i>Apis</i> spp.) counter attacks from hornets, an important predator, by the use of shimmering waves (Tan et al., <span>2012</span>) and defensive balling (Ono et al., <span>1995</span>); they also use grooming and other hygienic behaviors to suppress the spread of parasitic varroa mites (Harbo &amp; Harris, <span>2005</span>). Ants are also important consumers of honey, broods, and worker bees in beehives (Kern, <span>2017</span>), and it has been reported that some honey bee species inhibit the entry of ants into the hive through fan-blowing, a defensive behavior in which bees use wind pressure from fanning their wings to keep ants away from the hive entrance (Spangler &amp; Taer, <span>1970</span>). Because this behavior inhibits ant ingress, while avoiding direct contact between ants and the bees, it is thought to reduce the risk of injury or death of the defending bees (Seeley et al., <span>1982</span>). However, the Japanese honey bee (<i>Apis cerana japonica</i>), the Japanese subspecies of the eastern honey bee (<i>A. cerana</i>), repels ants by placing its wings directly in contact with intruding ants and flicking them away. Although this behavior has been briefly described in anecdotal accounts (Fujiwara et al., <span>2015</span>; Yoshida, <span>1997</span>), the detailed behavior sequence has remained unclear.</p><p>We filmed Japanese honey bee colonies being invaded by the Japanese pavement ant (<i>Tetramorium tsushimae</i>) with a high-speed camera (960 fps; DSC-RX10M4, Sony Group Corporation, Japan), and recorded honey bee defensive behaviors against the ants in detail. Our results show that the bees slap the ants through direct contact with their wings (Figure 1; Videos S1 and S2). Specifically, the worker bees executed the slap behavior by first tilting their bodies toward the ants, then flapping their wings while simultaneously turning their bodies. These observations demonstrate that the defensive behavior employed by Japanese honey bees against ants is clearly different from the noncontact fan-blowing behavior (Yang et al., <span>2010</span>) reported in other honey bees. We named this new defensive behavior “wing-slapping.”</p><p>In addition to the above observations, to identify whether wing-slapping behavior is only carried out against specific ant species and whether there are differences in effectiveness among ant species, we introduced ants to the vicinity of the hive entrance and determined the frequency of wing-slapping by Japanese honey bees. The observations were carried out in June and July 2023 on two colonies (A and B) of Japanese honey bees in Tsukuba City, Ibaraki Prefecture (36°02′54.1″ N, 140°06′54.8″ E). The Japanese honey bee colonies were raised in movable-frame hives with a platform of dimensions 50 × 260 mm at the hive entrance. Worker bees that remained on the entrance platform for more than 5 s were regarded as guard bees, and were marked with paint for identification. For the ant intruders, we selected three species from among the common Japanese ant species that are known to invade Japanese honey bee hives: the Japanese queenless ant <i>Pristomyrmex punctatus</i>, the Japanese pavement ant, and the Japanese wood ant <i>Formica japonica</i> (Miyano et al., <span>2001</span>; Okada, <span>1997</span>). These species were collected at the study site. Specimens were placed in a plastic cup with free access to the hive entrance. Then, the platform was filmed from directly above with a digital camera for 30 min. From the video, the behavior of marked guard bees was observed and recorded during periods when the distance between an ant and the center of the bee's head was 10 mm or less. In addition, the success rate of the wing-slaps was calculated. A wing-slap was deemed to be successful if it resulted in the targeted ant being flicked away.</p><p>Wing-slapping was the most commonly observed behavior performed by guard bees toward nearby ants (Figure 2a). However, western honey bees show distinct responses to different beetle species (Atkinson &amp; Ellis, <span>2011</span>), and more observations of Japanese honey bees responding to a variety of ant species are needed to understand whether their responses might also be species specific. In our study, whereas wing-slapping had a success rate of roughly one in two or three attempts in repelling intruding Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, the success rate against Japanese wood ants was relatively low (Figure 2b), suggesting the behavior might not be effective against all ant species. Given that Japanese wood ants have about twice the body length and move more quickly than the other species examined in the present study, it may have been difficult to successfully flick them away.</p><p>Wing-slapping behavior might have evolved among Japanese honey bees as a relatively low-energy, generic defense against the threat of ant incursion. Fan-blowing behavior is thought to function as a way for bees to avoid direct contact with predator ants (e.g., Asian weaver ants <i>Oecophylla smaragdina</i>; Seeley et al., <span>1982</span>). However, although Japanese ants, especially Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, will invade Japanese honey bee hives, they rarely bite and consume healthy workers in the hive (Okada, <span>1997</span>; Yoshida, <span>1997</span>; see Miyano et al., <span>2001</span> for an exception). Therefore, in Japan, where predator ants such as the Asian weaver ant are absent and the threat of direct predation by ants is low, noncontact defensive behaviors might not always be optimal. In other words, ants with low direct predation threat can be eliminated more efficiently (i.e., with lower energy cost) through direct contact rather than through generating wind pressure. To test this hypothesis, it will be necessary to investigate the aggression (predation ability) of ants and compare it against the defensive behaviors of bees in various areas. The behaviors employed to defend against ant incursion are yet to be identified for most honey bee species and subspecies. Such information, if collected in the future, would provide an interesting opportunity to examine the evolution of defensive behaviors among social insects through inter- and intraspecific comparison.</p><p>Anthropogenic range expansions can place honey bees in contact with predator species they have not evolved alongside, increasing the risk of predation. This risk has become a reality for some species, including the northern giant hornet (<i>Vespa mandarinia</i>) (Matsuura &amp; Sakagami, <span>1973</span>). The same logic would suggest that populations of the Japanese honey bee, which did not evolve alongside predatory ants, would suffer serious harm if exposed to nonnative ants that are capable of preying on bees. For example, big-headed ants prey upon healthy workers of western honey bees (Kern, <span>2017</span>; Yang et al., <span>2010</span>). Considering that big-headed ants are currently expanding their distribution in southern Japan (Yamane et al., <span>2019</span>), Japanese honey bees could come into frequent contact with them in the future. If Japanese honey bees respond to these nonnative predators with wing-slapping, which involves direct contact, they may be subjected to a higher risk of mortality than other honey bees that exhibit noncontact defensive responses. Invasive ants are a widespread problem around the world, and there is no doubt that new invasions will occur in the future. Therefore, the resulting harm to honey bees may become more severe over time.</p><p>Additionally, the defensive behaviors of honey bees against ants continue to raise numerous research questions, such as how bees detect ants, whether there are variations in the innate aptitude of individual honey bees for a given behavior, and whether the success rate of behavior increases with experience. Although ants are important consumers of bees and bee products (Kern, <span>2017</span>), defensive behaviors of bees against ants have received little attention, perhaps because these interactions are inconspicuous to human observers. Our study identifies a new defensive behavior, which we have named wing-slapping, and opens the door to further studies examining the evolution of defensive behaviors in social insects.</p><p>Yugo Seko and Kiyohito Morii collected and analyzed the data, took photographs and videos, and wrote the first draft. Yugo Seko, Kiyohito Morii, and Yoshiko Sakamoto designed the study and revised the manuscript.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":11484,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Ecology\",\"volume\":\"105 8\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":4.4000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-07-08\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ecy.4372\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Ecology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"93\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecy.4372\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"环境科学与生态学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"ECOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Ecology","FirstCategoryId":"93","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ecy.4372","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"ECOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

拍打翅膀的行为可能是日本蜜蜂进化出的一种能量相对较低的普通防御手段,用于抵御蚂蚁入侵的威胁。吹扇行为被认为是蜜蜂避免直接接触捕食蚂蚁的一种方式(例如,亚洲织蚁 Oecophylla smaragdina;Seeley 等人,1982 年)。然而,尽管日本蚂蚁,特别是日本无王蚁和日本铺路蚁,会入侵日本蜜蜂蜂巢,但它们很少咬伤和吃掉蜂巢中健康的工蜂(Okada,1997;Yoshida,1997;例外情况见 Miyano 等人,2001)。因此,在日本,没有亚洲织网蚁这样的天敌蚂蚁,蚂蚁直接捕食的威胁也很低,非接触防御行为不一定总是最佳的。换句话说,直接捕食威胁低的蚂蚁可以通过直接接触而不是产生风压的方式更有效地消灭(即能量成本更低)。为了验证这一假设,有必要调查蚂蚁的攻击性(捕食能力),并将其与不同地区蜜蜂的防御行为进行比较。大多数蜜蜂物种和亚种抵御蚂蚁入侵的行为尚未确定。人类活动范围的扩大可能会使蜜蜂与它们未曾共同进化的捕食者物种接触,从而增加被捕食的风险。这种风险已经成为某些物种的现实,包括北方大黄蜂(Vespa mandarinia)(Matsuura &amp; Sakagami, 1973)。同样的逻辑表明,没有与捕食性蚂蚁一起进化的日本蜜蜂种群,如果遇到能够捕食蜜蜂的非本地蚂蚁,也会遭受严重伤害。例如,大头蚁会捕食西方蜜蜂的健康工蜂(Kern,2017;Yang 等人,2010)。考虑到大头蚁目前正在扩大其在日本南部的分布(Yamane 等人,2019 年),日本蜜蜂将来可能会经常与它们接触。如果日本蜜蜂对这些非本地捕食者的反应是拍打翅膀,即直接接触,那么与其他表现出非接触防御反应的蜜蜂相比,它们的死亡风险可能会更高。蚂蚁入侵是全世界普遍存在的问题,毫无疑问,未来还会有新的蚂蚁入侵。此外,蜜蜂对蚂蚁的防御行为仍然提出了许多研究问题,如蜜蜂如何发现蚂蚁,蜜蜂个体对特定行为的先天能力是否存在差异,以及行为的成功率是否会随着经验的增加而增加。虽然蚂蚁是蜜蜂和蜂产品的重要消费者(Kern,2017 年),但蜜蜂对蚂蚁的防御行为却很少受到关注,也许是因为这些互动对人类观察者来说并不明显。我们的研究发现了一种新的防御行为,并将其命名为拍翅,这为进一步研究社会性昆虫防御行为的进化打开了大门。Yugo Seko、Kiyohito Morii 和 Yoshiko Sakamoto 设计了这项研究并修改了手稿。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。

Wing-slapping: A defensive behavior by honey bees against ants

Wing-slapping: A defensive behavior by honey bees against ants

Eusocial organisms are group living, and their nests, which are inhabited by numerous eggs, larvae, and adults, are attractive feeding grounds for predators and parasites. To effectively defend the nest against such natural enemies, eusocial organisms have evolved specific defensive behaviors. For example, honey bees (Apis spp.) counter attacks from hornets, an important predator, by the use of shimmering waves (Tan et al., 2012) and defensive balling (Ono et al., 1995); they also use grooming and other hygienic behaviors to suppress the spread of parasitic varroa mites (Harbo & Harris, 2005). Ants are also important consumers of honey, broods, and worker bees in beehives (Kern, 2017), and it has been reported that some honey bee species inhibit the entry of ants into the hive through fan-blowing, a defensive behavior in which bees use wind pressure from fanning their wings to keep ants away from the hive entrance (Spangler & Taer, 1970). Because this behavior inhibits ant ingress, while avoiding direct contact between ants and the bees, it is thought to reduce the risk of injury or death of the defending bees (Seeley et al., 1982). However, the Japanese honey bee (Apis cerana japonica), the Japanese subspecies of the eastern honey bee (A. cerana), repels ants by placing its wings directly in contact with intruding ants and flicking them away. Although this behavior has been briefly described in anecdotal accounts (Fujiwara et al., 2015; Yoshida, 1997), the detailed behavior sequence has remained unclear.

We filmed Japanese honey bee colonies being invaded by the Japanese pavement ant (Tetramorium tsushimae) with a high-speed camera (960 fps; DSC-RX10M4, Sony Group Corporation, Japan), and recorded honey bee defensive behaviors against the ants in detail. Our results show that the bees slap the ants through direct contact with their wings (Figure 1; Videos S1 and S2). Specifically, the worker bees executed the slap behavior by first tilting their bodies toward the ants, then flapping their wings while simultaneously turning their bodies. These observations demonstrate that the defensive behavior employed by Japanese honey bees against ants is clearly different from the noncontact fan-blowing behavior (Yang et al., 2010) reported in other honey bees. We named this new defensive behavior “wing-slapping.”

In addition to the above observations, to identify whether wing-slapping behavior is only carried out against specific ant species and whether there are differences in effectiveness among ant species, we introduced ants to the vicinity of the hive entrance and determined the frequency of wing-slapping by Japanese honey bees. The observations were carried out in June and July 2023 on two colonies (A and B) of Japanese honey bees in Tsukuba City, Ibaraki Prefecture (36°02′54.1″ N, 140°06′54.8″ E). The Japanese honey bee colonies were raised in movable-frame hives with a platform of dimensions 50 × 260 mm at the hive entrance. Worker bees that remained on the entrance platform for more than 5 s were regarded as guard bees, and were marked with paint for identification. For the ant intruders, we selected three species from among the common Japanese ant species that are known to invade Japanese honey bee hives: the Japanese queenless ant Pristomyrmex punctatus, the Japanese pavement ant, and the Japanese wood ant Formica japonica (Miyano et al., 2001; Okada, 1997). These species were collected at the study site. Specimens were placed in a plastic cup with free access to the hive entrance. Then, the platform was filmed from directly above with a digital camera for 30 min. From the video, the behavior of marked guard bees was observed and recorded during periods when the distance between an ant and the center of the bee's head was 10 mm or less. In addition, the success rate of the wing-slaps was calculated. A wing-slap was deemed to be successful if it resulted in the targeted ant being flicked away.

Wing-slapping was the most commonly observed behavior performed by guard bees toward nearby ants (Figure 2a). However, western honey bees show distinct responses to different beetle species (Atkinson & Ellis, 2011), and more observations of Japanese honey bees responding to a variety of ant species are needed to understand whether their responses might also be species specific. In our study, whereas wing-slapping had a success rate of roughly one in two or three attempts in repelling intruding Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, the success rate against Japanese wood ants was relatively low (Figure 2b), suggesting the behavior might not be effective against all ant species. Given that Japanese wood ants have about twice the body length and move more quickly than the other species examined in the present study, it may have been difficult to successfully flick them away.

Wing-slapping behavior might have evolved among Japanese honey bees as a relatively low-energy, generic defense against the threat of ant incursion. Fan-blowing behavior is thought to function as a way for bees to avoid direct contact with predator ants (e.g., Asian weaver ants Oecophylla smaragdina; Seeley et al., 1982). However, although Japanese ants, especially Japanese queenless ants and Japanese pavement ants, will invade Japanese honey bee hives, they rarely bite and consume healthy workers in the hive (Okada, 1997; Yoshida, 1997; see Miyano et al., 2001 for an exception). Therefore, in Japan, where predator ants such as the Asian weaver ant are absent and the threat of direct predation by ants is low, noncontact defensive behaviors might not always be optimal. In other words, ants with low direct predation threat can be eliminated more efficiently (i.e., with lower energy cost) through direct contact rather than through generating wind pressure. To test this hypothesis, it will be necessary to investigate the aggression (predation ability) of ants and compare it against the defensive behaviors of bees in various areas. The behaviors employed to defend against ant incursion are yet to be identified for most honey bee species and subspecies. Such information, if collected in the future, would provide an interesting opportunity to examine the evolution of defensive behaviors among social insects through inter- and intraspecific comparison.

Anthropogenic range expansions can place honey bees in contact with predator species they have not evolved alongside, increasing the risk of predation. This risk has become a reality for some species, including the northern giant hornet (Vespa mandarinia) (Matsuura & Sakagami, 1973). The same logic would suggest that populations of the Japanese honey bee, which did not evolve alongside predatory ants, would suffer serious harm if exposed to nonnative ants that are capable of preying on bees. For example, big-headed ants prey upon healthy workers of western honey bees (Kern, 2017; Yang et al., 2010). Considering that big-headed ants are currently expanding their distribution in southern Japan (Yamane et al., 2019), Japanese honey bees could come into frequent contact with them in the future. If Japanese honey bees respond to these nonnative predators with wing-slapping, which involves direct contact, they may be subjected to a higher risk of mortality than other honey bees that exhibit noncontact defensive responses. Invasive ants are a widespread problem around the world, and there is no doubt that new invasions will occur in the future. Therefore, the resulting harm to honey bees may become more severe over time.

Additionally, the defensive behaviors of honey bees against ants continue to raise numerous research questions, such as how bees detect ants, whether there are variations in the innate aptitude of individual honey bees for a given behavior, and whether the success rate of behavior increases with experience. Although ants are important consumers of bees and bee products (Kern, 2017), defensive behaviors of bees against ants have received little attention, perhaps because these interactions are inconspicuous to human observers. Our study identifies a new defensive behavior, which we have named wing-slapping, and opens the door to further studies examining the evolution of defensive behaviors in social insects.

Yugo Seko and Kiyohito Morii collected and analyzed the data, took photographs and videos, and wrote the first draft. Yugo Seko, Kiyohito Morii, and Yoshiko Sakamoto designed the study and revised the manuscript.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Ecology
Ecology 环境科学-生态学
CiteScore
8.30
自引率
2.10%
发文量
332
审稿时长
3 months
期刊介绍: Ecology publishes articles that report on the basic elements of ecological research. Emphasis is placed on concise, clear articles documenting important ecological phenomena. The journal publishes a broad array of research that includes a rapidly expanding envelope of subject matter, techniques, approaches, and concepts: paleoecology through present-day phenomena; evolutionary, population, physiological, community, and ecosystem ecology, as well as biogeochemistry; inclusive of descriptive, comparative, experimental, mathematical, statistical, and interdisciplinary approaches.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信