{"title":"Neuroethology of Zooplankton","authors":"T. Bullock","doi":"10.1201/9780203733615-1","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Whereas the neural analysis of behavior of planktonic species and stages has been relatively neglected, we have many clues that it is going to be rich, diverse and interesting. The aims of this contribution are to defend that statement, with selected examples, and to suggest that neural analysis, particularly sensory physiology, has great explanatory power of ecologically significant behavior. I have to begin with a personal note about plankton, recalling the lasting impression made long ago by a film on invertebrates in the Arctic where scyphomedusan jellyfish were pulsing at a rate well within the range familiar in summer temperate waters, warmer by 20o C. I must have been influenced by this observation and my own experiences in a study of the neural basis of fluctuations in the rate of pulsation of medusae (Bullock 1943), some of which was made in December 1941 in Pensacola, where my wife and I collected Rhopilema cruising at random in the Sound, stopped now and then by Army bridge guards concerned about saboteurs in that first fortnight after Pearl Harbor. At any rate, by the early fifties about half of my laboratory group was devoted to the physiological ecology of temperature acclimation in marine invertebrates. That field, which I left in the early sixties, still offers a challenge in the ecologically fundamental question of why some species are able to acclimate much more than others. The proposal I made in 1955, that different rates in the same organism acclimate to different degrees, resulting in greater disharmony in some species than others, may still be viable and most likely applies to rate processes in sensory and central nervous functions, among others. Medusae are large animals, relatively, although generally treated as planktonic. The first reaction from most workers when neurophysiology of plankton is mentioned concerns their small size or gelatinous nature. The first message I bring is not new but also not widely appreciated.","PeriodicalId":360564,"journal":{"name":"Zooplankton: sensory ecology and physiology","volume":"38 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Zooplankton: sensory ecology and physiology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1201/9780203733615-1","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Abstract
Whereas the neural analysis of behavior of planktonic species and stages has been relatively neglected, we have many clues that it is going to be rich, diverse and interesting. The aims of this contribution are to defend that statement, with selected examples, and to suggest that neural analysis, particularly sensory physiology, has great explanatory power of ecologically significant behavior. I have to begin with a personal note about plankton, recalling the lasting impression made long ago by a film on invertebrates in the Arctic where scyphomedusan jellyfish were pulsing at a rate well within the range familiar in summer temperate waters, warmer by 20o C. I must have been influenced by this observation and my own experiences in a study of the neural basis of fluctuations in the rate of pulsation of medusae (Bullock 1943), some of which was made in December 1941 in Pensacola, where my wife and I collected Rhopilema cruising at random in the Sound, stopped now and then by Army bridge guards concerned about saboteurs in that first fortnight after Pearl Harbor. At any rate, by the early fifties about half of my laboratory group was devoted to the physiological ecology of temperature acclimation in marine invertebrates. That field, which I left in the early sixties, still offers a challenge in the ecologically fundamental question of why some species are able to acclimate much more than others. The proposal I made in 1955, that different rates in the same organism acclimate to different degrees, resulting in greater disharmony in some species than others, may still be viable and most likely applies to rate processes in sensory and central nervous functions, among others. Medusae are large animals, relatively, although generally treated as planktonic. The first reaction from most workers when neurophysiology of plankton is mentioned concerns their small size or gelatinous nature. The first message I bring is not new but also not widely appreciated.
尽管浮游生物物种和阶段行为的神经分析相对被忽视,但我们有许多线索表明,它将是丰富、多样和有趣的。这篇文章的目的是通过选择的例子来捍卫这一说法,并表明神经分析,特别是感觉生理学,对生态上重要的行为有很大的解释能力。开始我有个人注意浮游生物,回忆起很久以前电影的持久的印象在无脊椎动物在北极scyphomedusan水母是脉冲速率在夏季温带水域范围内的熟悉,温暖20 o c。我一定是受到这个观察和我自己的经验的神经基础的研究脉动速度的波动母体"(布洛克1943),其中一些是在1941年12月在彭萨科拉,在那里,我和我的妻子在海湾里随意地收集罗皮勒玛,在珍珠港事件后的头两周,陆军桥梁警卫担心会有破坏者,不时地把它拦下来。无论如何,到五十年代初,我的实验室小组中大约有一半的人致力于海洋无脊椎动物温度适应的生理生态学。这个领域,我在60年代早期就离开了,仍然对生态学的基本问题提出了挑战,为什么有些物种比其他物种更能适应环境。我在1955年提出的建议,即同一生物体中不同的速率适应程度不同,导致某些物种比其他物种更不和谐,这可能仍然是可行的,而且很可能适用于感觉和中枢神经功能的速率过程,以及其他。水母是相对较大的动物,尽管通常被视为浮游生物。当提到浮游生物的神经生理学时,大多数工人的第一反应是关注它们的小尺寸或凝胶性。我带来的第一个信息并不新鲜,但也没有得到广泛的认可。